Study
Unit OC3 - Communication and the Naked Ape
Copyright Notice: This material was
written and published in Wales by Derek J. Smith (Chartered Engineer). It forms
part of a multifile e-learning resource, and subject only to acknowledging
Derek J. Smith's rights under international copyright law to be identified as
author may be freely downloaded and printed off in single complete copies
solely for the purposes of private study and/or review. Commercial exploitation
rights are reserved. The remote hyperlinks have been selected for the academic
appropriacy of their contents; they were free of offensive and litigious
content when selected, and will be periodically checked to have remained so. Copyright © 2001-2018, Derek J. Smith.
|
First published online 08:54 BST 19th April 2001,
Copyright Derek J. Smith (Chartered Engineer). This version [2.0 - copyright] 09:00 BST 4th July
2018.
Unit Outcomes: This study unit looks at modern approaches to the study of instinct, and then searches for intrusions of instinctively generated behaviour into the rationality of our daily organisational lives. When you have completed it, you will be able to deploy with enhanced confidence and accuracy the specific skills and vocabulary listed below:
Specific Skills |
Vocabulary |
1. To use selected vocabulary from the sciences of ethology and zoology. |
animat; appeasement gesture; central pattern generator (CPG); chain reflex; dominance hierarchy; fixed action pattern (FAP); innate releasing mechanism (IRM); kinesis; releasing ceremony; sign stimulus; taxis; threat gesture |
2. To review early attempts to apply ethological analyses to human behaviour. |
body language; human zoo; kinesics; manwatching; naked ape; nonverbal communication (NVC); personal space; pinocchio science; primatology; proxemics; rhetorical devices; silent signalling; social skills |
3. To analyse organisational pecking order, and apply an in-group / out-group analysis in an organisational context. |
in-group vs out-group; limbic system; pecking order; social grooming; social order |
Unit Structure
This unit contains three short lessons, each contributing to the overall unit outcomes, each with its own hyperlinked support material, and each with its own additional reading and tutorial task(s). Here is the learning sequence:
Lesson OC3.1: The Science of Ethology
Lesson OC3.3: Social Order and Aggression
Lesson
OC3.1: The Science of Ethology
The scientific study of animal behaviour became
popular in the late 19th Century, as part of a surge in interest in science in
general. Here are some background readings you might find useful:
Romanes' views (eg. Romanes, 1886) were particularly controversial, because
they freely attributed human qualities to lower animals. For example he remarks
upon the "pride, sense of dignity, and self-respect" of dogs (p439),
and includes an (admittedly second-hand) account of a pet boa-constrictor which
moped when separated from its owners but which "sprang upon
them with delight" on their return (p261). Most of Romanes' contemporaries set out to demonstrate the very
opposite, namely that animals were frequently little more than biological
robots executing biological computer programs (although they did not use those
precise metaphors, because neither cybernetics nor computing had themselves yet
been invented). Led by Lloyd Morgan, they tried to exclude anthropomorphism and
teleology from their explanations.
Key Concepts - Anthropomorphism and Teleology: Anthropomorphism (literally man-form-ism) is an assertion of
human characteristics in inanimate objects or subhuman species. Thus if you
talk deeply and meaningfully to your canary or swear at your car when it fails
to start, then you are elevating those objects to humanlike status. Similarly,
if you conceive of animals as feeling human emotions such as love, regret,
compassion, etc. A teleology is an assertion of
voluntary prior purpose, as in "I'm studying for my exams" or
"That fruit fly flew from A to B to get a drink." As a result,
teleological explanations are frowned upon by science, (a) because prior
purpose is usually not provable, and (b) because there is the suspicion that
they are convenient oversimplifications. Animals do not mate in order to
continue their blood line, for example, they just exist and respond when
programmed to respond.
The greatest boost
to Romanes' critics came from repeated demonstrations
in the early 20th Century of how easy it was to catch nature out and make a
fool of it. What people liked to think of as instincts often turned out to be
very easy to fragment. Instincts are complexes of individually quite simple
behaviours, which seem to be held together by little - if any - overriding
control. When that control is left unimpaired, all the individual behavioural
elements are discharged one by one and in the right order, and the
"instinct" is fulfilled. On the other hand, when circumstances (or
the investigator) interfere with this process, the natural chain of events is
broken and the instinct fails. Here are some specific and highly illustrative
examples of animal behaviour, all of which should help discourage teleological
and anthropomorphic explanations:
·
Fabre (1911) took some caterpillars of a species whose
"instincts" were to follow the silken path trailed out by their
leader, and intervened manually to form them into a ring, whereupon the animals
went round and round in circles for a solid week! Caterpillars, in other words,
do not know much about their world. They just mechanically follow an inbuilt
program.
·
Volkelt (1914)
studied the hunting behaviour of funnel-web spiders. Normally, these spiders
lie in wait for their prey at the end of a silken tube built into the centre of
their web. When an insect becomes entangled in the web they dash out of the
central funnel, kill it, and retrieve it into their lair. However, when a fly
is put directly into the mouth of the funnel the spider adopts defensive rather
than aggressive behaviour. Spiders, in other words, do not know much about
flies. They, too, just mechanically follow an inbuilt program.
·
Barrows (1915) investigated what constituted the effective
stimulus for prey attack in the web-spinning spider Epeira,
and found it to be the web vibrations caused by the struggling captive. Upon
sensing these vibrations the spider would - if not already there - make its way
to the centre of the web, feel for which one of the "spokes" was vibrating
most, and then make its way out to the prey. False vibrations were equally
effective, provided the rate and amplitude were within appropriately lifelike
limits. Attack behaviour was then delivered more or less regardless to what was
found in the web, with a vibrating straw being bitten and wrapped up in silk.
Olfactory, visual, and auditory cues were ineffective. Again, spiders do not
seem to know much about flies. They, too, just mechanically follow an inbuilt
program.
·
Hingston (1928)
described the foraging behaviour of the dung-beetle. These insects work in
pairs to shape a bolus of dung out of animal droppings, and then roll it to a
storage site to bury it. If, upon their arrival at the dung, they are given a
ready-made dung ball, they ignore it. Instinct has given them a complex
program to execute and they are not free to take this particular short-cut, no
matter how sensible it might seem. Moreover, if having once made the ball
they have it taken away from them during the rolling phase, they will accept a
replacement without question and carry on rolling it. However, if having had
their original ball taken away from them they are instead put back upon the
dung pile, they will not do the sensible thing and make another ball, choosing
instead to keep looking around for the lost one. Again, they just do what their
programming tells them to do.
[A
more recent study by Melchers (1963) adds that if the silk is removed from a
spider's silk glands it will nevertheless go through its normal 6400-movement
web-spinning sequence even though nothing is actually being produced!]
The main
explanatory concepts which emerged from half a century's worth of observational
data such as these were as follows:
Glossary 1 -
Early Animal Psychology (1886 - 1934)
Note: These entries are in alphabetic order, so you may have to read them through two or three times to get their full message. |
Chain Reflexes: There is a limit to what can be accomplished with a simple reflex, so if an organism wants to be really clever it needs even more advanced types of behaviour; it needs a logically progressive sequence of behaviours. The term chain reflex was coined by Loeb (1890) to describe the concatenation (ie. chaining together) of simple reflex responses into these more complex pieces of behaviour. The successful completion of one reflex response provides the stimulus for another. Take, for example, the act of a toad darting out its tongue to catch a fly. The first link in the chain would be between vision and tongue protrusion, and the second would be between the resulting sensation on the tongue and the act of swallowing. Many instinctive behaviours are chains of stimuli and responses just like this, and frequently involve two or more participating organisms. Unfortunately, this sort of approach fails adequately to explain behaviours which show any form of variety. [Compare the concept of the releasing ceremony in Glossary 2.] |
Innate: "Existing in a person (or organism) from birth" (O.E.D.). Some authors prefer to call unlearned behaviours "innate" rather than "instinctive", because of the problems with the concept of instinct (see next). |
Instincts: An instinct is "an innate propensity in organised beings (esp. in the lower animals), varying with the species, and manifesting itself in acts which appear to be rational, but are performed without conscious design or intentional adaptation of means to ends. Also, the faculty supposed to be involved in this operation" (O.E.D.). They must also be complex, adaptive, and common to all members of a species. Upon analysis, however, the relationship between an instinctive motivation and the behaviours it provokes is far from straightforward. It is therefore scientifically dangerous to speak, say, of an instinct to reproduce. Instead, when we look at complex behaviours such as foraging, courtship, mating, nesting, and parenting, we are forced to treat these as somehow unrelated clusters of smaller units of behaviour. Which presents us with a serious - and as yet unresolved - contradiction: on the one hand such behaviour is complex enough to seem almost consciously purposeful, yet on the other hand it cannot be, for it is demonstrably fragile. |
Kineses: (Sing. kinesis.) Kineses (pronounced "kine-ees-eez") are the simple, undirected (compare taxes below), and entirely automatic movements of simple organisms. Woodlice, for example, become more active as the humidity in their environment increases. Similarly, protozoa will escape from water which is too hot or too cold by repeatedly backing off and turning until they reach water of the desired temperature, or die in the attempt. |
Kuhn's Classification: [See firstly taxes.] Kuhn (1929) formulated what has since become known as Kuhn's classification, which starts by dividing behaviour into phobotaxes (avoidance reactions) and topotaxes (approach reactions). The approach reactions can then be divided in turn into (a) tropotaxes, wherein the organism turns so that symmetrically placed sensory organs are equally stimulated, (b) telotaxes, wherein the organism turns towards the source of stimulation, (c) menotaxes, wherein the organism turns so that a certain distribution of stimulation is maintained, and (d) mnemotaxes, wherein the organism turns to a position (or series of positions) previously memorised as appropriate to the stimulus in question (eg. "homing" behaviour). |
Orienting Reactions: Alternative name for taxes. So named because taxes do little more than direct an organism either towards or away from a particular stimulus. |
Reflexes: A reflex is a wholly involuntary piece of behaviour which takes place because the nervous system is "wired up" in such a way that it cannot not take place. Reflexes are responsible for many automatic behaviours, especially those which have a biological "safety" aspect to them, such as blinking, coughing, gagging, and rapid limb withdrawal from pain. |
Taxes: (Sing. taxis.) Taxes (pronounced "tax-eez") are the simple, directed (unlike kineses), and entirely automatic locomotor orientation reactions of simple organisms. A winkle, for example, will move away from a light source and upwards against gravity (except when upside down when it moves instead towards the light source). Similarly, a fruit fly will orient itself towards the wind-borne fermentation products - the alcohols, etc. - of decaying fruit: it will simply fly upwind whenever it smells a drink, and that - given the nature of the world - is all it needs to do. Many species find mates the same way, by flying upwind whenever they detect the airborne sex hormones (pheromones) of distant potential partners. It all looks very purposeful to the casual observer, but the behaviours are actually just orienting responses. Depending upon the energy source involved, taxes can be classed as phototaxes (light-driven), geotaxes (gravity-driven), chemotaxes (chemical-driven), rheotaxes (current-flow driven), etc. [See also Kuhn's classification.] |
Thinking and Problem Solving: This is the application of higher cognitive functions (such as intelligence, insight, creativity, and problem solving) to the problems of behaviour. This is where an organism's abilities start to move beyond the merely instinctual, and into the realms of the rational. There is much debate, however, (a) as to what rationality actually is, and (b) how much of it you will find even in the self-proclaimed rational minds of humans, let alone in subhuman species. |
Tropisms: This term was coined by Loeb (1888, 1890) to describe the wholly involuntary behaviour of organisms lacking a nervous system. Just as plants can turn towards the sun using non-nervous mechanisms, so, too, is much simple animal behaviour unwilled. It just happens. To be classified as a tropism, the relationship between the stimulus and the response must be rigid and resulting directly from the action of some external energy source. Tropisms can be subcategorised by energy type and direction of response as follows: Geotropisms: positive - turn towards the pull of gravity; negative - turn away from the pull of gravity, Heliotropisms (or phototropisms): positive: turn towards the light; negative - turn away from the light, Thermotropisms: positive: turn towards heat; negative - turn away from heat, and Rheotropisms: positive: turn upstream (in air or water); negative - turn downstream. In animals with nervous systems, this type of behaviour is provided by reflexes. |
To summarise
the situation so far .....
So in fact an
organism which is capable of taxes is actually beginning to get quite
"clever". For example, the entire life of a tick requires only three
reflexes and one taxis. Thus:
"A
tick may stay clinging to a twig, perhaps for months, until exposed to a low
concentration of butyric acid, a component of the smell of a mammal, released
from skin glands. It then drops, falling on to the mammal, attaches itself to
the mammal's skin, and gorges itself with blood. When fully fed, it may have a
strong tendency to climb upwards when on vertical surfaces, but this would be
largely due to the fact that its body is swollen with blood behind the region
of the legs, so that the mechanical force of gravity will keep the tick's body
oriented in an upward direction." (Walker, 1987:20.)
[At this point students might find it very informative to glance at the sort
of behaviours robot termites can achieve, working to only one or two rules - click here.]
Further
theoretical advances were made in the mid-20th Century by a new school known as
the ethologists. The defining characteristic of this new approach was
that it set out to study animals in their natural conditions, and its founders
were an Austrian named Konrad Lorenz and a Dutchman named Niko
Tinbergen. Ethology's key rule is that you must observe, thus:
".....the
observation of all there is to be observed in the behaviour of a species
must go before the quest for explanation." (Lorenz,
1950:233; emphasis original.)
Lorenz and
Tinbergen's first joint paper, describing the egg-retrieving behaviour of the
grey lag goose, was published in 1938, but the school was not really free to
flourish until after World War II. The breakthrough came with a 1949 symposium on
animal behaviour held by the Society for Experimental Biology, and with the
publication of The Study of Instinct (Tinbergen, 1951) and King
Solomon's Ring (Lorenz, 1952). The area was subsequently developed by such
workers as Hinde, Thorpe, Eibl-Eibesfeldt, and Scott, and has also helped to
shape the "popular zoology" of the likes of Desmond Morris and David
Attenborough.
Ethologists have
studied the behaviour of many species, most famously geese, gulls, fish [for
the story of the three-spined stickleback, click
here], and mammals [for the story of
the confused polecat, click here].
In then interpreting their observations, the ethologists took the earlier
concept of the chain reflex, and improved upon it, keeping the sequentiality of behaviour but pointing out just how
complex the underlying mechanisms probably were. Thus:
"What
formerly was very simply conceived of as 'an instinct' is shown by the advance
of analysis to be a very complex mechanism of very distinct and very different
constituents, such as endogenous automatisms, releasing mechanisms, taxes, kineses, and, maybe, quite a number of further as yet
unrecognised particulate functions" (Lorenz, 1950:261).
Indeed, perhaps
the ethologists' most enduring contribution to psychology has been the
vocabulary they introduced. Lorenz's use of the term "releasing
mechanism" (above) is particularly valuable, because it emphasises the
observable fact that each link in an instinctive chain of behaviours seems to
have its own effective stimulus, provided in some way by the satisfactory
completion of the immediately preceding piece of behaviour, and often involving
more than one participant.
Key Vocabulary: The usual name for the "releasing", or
"effective", stimulus in an innate stimulus-response pairing is sign
stimulus. This term was popularised by Tinbergen (1951), but actually
attributed by him to Russell (1943). It is synonymous with the term releaser
previously introduced by Lorenz (1937). The response to the
sign stimulus - ie. the
specific attack manoeuvre, etc. - is called a fixed action pattern (FAP),
and the ritual by which sign stimuli and FAPs interact and cross-stimulate each
other (much along the lines of the chain reflex described above) is called a releasing
ceremony.
Here is some more
of that vocabulary. Study it carefully, for these are the concepts we will
shortly be applying to human behaviour in organisations:
Glossary 2 -
Modern Animal Psychology (1938 - Present)
Note: These entries are in alphabetic order, so you may have to read them through two or three times to get their full message. |
Alpha (Fe)male: The animal at the top of a dominance hierarchy. |
Animats: During the 1990s, there arose a lot of interest in what we might describe as "comparative robotics". Many cognitive science departments began experimenting with small robots, known generically as "animats", in which simple behavioural strategies could be programmed. Some remarkably lifelike "behaviours" have resulted. It follows that it may well become easier to understand a biological instinct once we are able to program an animat to behave in the same way, and one of the research teams (under Pattie Maes of MIT) has already identified two major barriers to achieving this. These are (a) selecting actions from a repertoire, and (b) allowing that repertoire to be improved by learning - precisely the same problems, note, that comparative psychology has been wrestling with for so many years! |
Appeasement Gestures: [See firstly threat gesture.] The partner to the threat gesture is what is known as the appeasement gesture. This is a sign of submission, and can be readily illustrated by the social order behaviour of carnivores. In wolves and wild dogs, for example, displays of aggression towards conspecifics are regular and easy to provoke (try taking a bone away from your pet dog). And yet this aggression results in very little actual bodily harm and very few deaths. This anomaly is explained by the operation of appeasement displays. Canines threaten by baring their fangs and erecting their ears, but if the target of that threat is happy to concede defeat it can readily defuse the situation and prevent further attack. All it needs to do is lay its own ears back and turn its head away to hide its own fangs and present instead the (vulnerable) nape of its neck. Other common submission behaviours include crouching, slinking, turning away, dropping the tail, bobbing the upper body, patting, hugging, and grooming. An appeasement gesture is thus a sign stimulus produced by one animal in response to a sign stimulus from a conspecific, and designed to signal acceptance of that conspecific's dominance. [See more detailed discussion in Lesson 3.] |
Central Pattern Generator (CPG): There has been increasing interest since the 1980s as to how instinctive behaviour is encoded within the nervous system, and the explanation of choice is that there are innate neuronal circuits specific to the fixed action patterns in a given species' behavioural repertoire. These are called central pattern generators (CPGs). The term is "generally taken to mean a centrally located system capable of generating, in the absence of input from peripheral receptors, a rhythmic motor pattern similar to that occurring in the normal animal" (Pearson, 1985:307). |
Displacement Activities: A displacement activity is a behaviour which is generated by external circumstances, but which is not appropriate to it. The concept of displacement activities was introduced by early ethologists, given its name by Armstrong (1947), and popularised by Tinbergen (1951, 1952), and the phenomenon can show itself in two superficially different ways, namely thwartings and intrusions. Either way, the suspicion is that some form of undischarged drive energy reaches such a level that in the absence of an appropriate goal object something else will trigger one or more of the usual fixed action patterns. Displacement activities are common in birds and fishes, and suggestions of them can also seen in higher animals, including humans. They are a sign of an underlying general arousal of autonomic systems, and may be thought of as indicating some level of competition between the central programs responsible for instinctive behaviours. Above all, they indicate that the relationship between a behaviour and its releaser is not as rigid as might have been thought. |
Dominance Hierarchy: A dominance hierarchy is the seniority ranking of the individual animals in a socially organised species. Such hierarchies are typical of carnivore packs, primate troops and tribes, and ungulate herds, and are enforced by prolonged competition for mating and feeding resources. They are established and maintained by the use of threat gestures by those wishing to assert dominance, and by submission gestures by those about to give it up. The animal at the top of the hierarchy is known as the alpha (fe)male. |
Drive: Behaviour is frequently a function of the internal hormonal state of the organism. The sign stimuli which operate during stickleback courting, for example, are only effective when the animals are in their breeding condition. At other times of year, they are ineffective. The concept of "internal hormonal state" and "drive" are therefore closely related, even though the former is a biochemical fact and the latter merely a convenient hypothetical construct. Lorenz saw drive as being like the water level building up in a reservoir until the time comes for it to be released or discharged by consummatory behaviour. As a specific physical need increases (for water, food, sex, etc.) so too does a readiness to execute the appropriate type(s) of behaviour (drinking, eating, copulating, etc.). This period is characterised by increasingly frequent intention movements and vacuum activities. Satisfactory consummation of the behaviour leads to reduction in the physical need, discharge of the "reservoir", and discontinuation of the behaviour. |
Fixed Action
Pattern (FAP): A fixed action
pattern is the output element of one of the sequence of stimulus-response
pairings making up a releasing ceremony. It is common to insist that a
piece of behaviour meets the following criteria before it can be classed as
an FAP:
In the final analysis, the necessary muscle contractions must result in some way from the activation of a central pattern generator (or hierarchy thereof) somewhere in the nervous system. |
Imprinting: Imprinting is the instinctive attachment of a newly born animal to the adult it first encounters. It is an area in which some challenging observations have been made over the years. Spalding (1873) was the first to note that 2- or 3-day chicks would readily start to follow a moving object regardless of that object's true biological appropriacy. The phenomenon was termed "imprinting" by Oskar Heinroth (1911), and "exposure learning" by Sluckin (1964). And again it is an instinct which can easily be fooled. Without food reinforcement, for example, birds will imprint upon balls, cardboard boxes, matchboxes, etc., and in one of the most famous ethological studies of all Lorenz (1937) clearly demonstrated how newly hatched grey lag geese would "adopt" him as their "mother", provided only that he was the thing they saw first upon emerging from the egg. |
Innate Releasing Mechanism (IRM): The IRM (or the angeborene auslösendes Schema in Lorenz's native German) was the term Lorenz (1937) used to describe the sort of central stimulus filtering observed in instinctive behaviour. The concept implies a central motivational state [see drive], IRMs to detect the appropriate sign stimuli, and the ability then to activate central pattern generators to deliver the corresponding fixed action patterns. All of these elements have to be mutually adapted if the combination is to be biologically adaptive. |
Intention Movement: An intention movement (Intentionsbewegung) is a premature attempt at a fully consummatory piece of behaviour. Such movements occur during the build-up of a particular drive, and indicate what Lorenz calls the "mood" of an animal. In his words, they are ".....nothing but a slight hint of a certain innate behaviour pattern, as will occur whenever action-specific excitation only reaches a very low level of intensity" (Lorenz, 1950:242). [For further discussion, click here.] |
Intrusions: A type of displacement activity [compare thwartings]. This is where an element of behaviour "B" suddenly crops up in the middle of behaviour "A". Manning (1967) gives the example of momentary ground pecking (feeding) behaviour during a fight between two cockerels. Similarly, an incubating tern will make a few brief preening movements just before it flies off to escape an intruder. |
Killing Bite: This is the name given to animals' stock-in-trade hunting bites. In polecats it is to the nape of the victim's neck, and in big cats it is to the windpipe. The behaviour itself is a potent mixture of innate and learned skills. [For fuller detail, click here] |
Pecking Order: Another name for dominance hierarchy. (Applicable strictly only to birds, but loosely to any species, including humans.) [See more detailed discussion in Lesson 3.] |
Reciprocal Altruism: There has recently been much study of altruistic (helping) behaviour in both man and animals. Hamilton (1964) was one of the first to point out that in evolutionary terms there was often a great deal of sense in non-selfish behaviour. Even if you were hungry it could make sense to share what little food you had. Cooperation, especially in the long run, was a winning strategy. This tendency (Morris, 1969, calls it the "cooperative urge") can be seen in many places. Goodall (1986) notes that when faced by predators monkeys will sacrifice their own life to save their infants, Morris (1969), in his analysis of the leadership behaviours of baboons, includes protection of the weak as one of the 10 essential roles, and Wilkinson (1983) has shown that vampire bats will share a blood meal if a kin bat has failed to feed. [For further examples, click here.] |
Releaser (or "Releasing Stimulus"): Same thing as sign stimulus. |
Releasing Ceremony: This is a logically related succession of separate stimulus-response exchanges, similar to the chain reflex described in Glossary 1. In Tinbergen's analysis of stickleback reproduction, for example, four releasing ceremonies are identified - (1) staking a territory, (2) nesting, (3) courting, and (4) hatching - with the transition from one to another being controlled by hormonal mechanisms [to refresh your memory, click here]. |
Sign
Stimulus: A sign stimulus is the
input element of one of the sequence of stimulus-response pairings making up
a releasing ceremony. Sign stimuli are often regarded as relying for
their detection on relatively central innate releasing mechanisms, but
in fact can be accounted for by a range of input mechanisms, both peripheral
and central. At the peripheral end of this spectrum of explanation, an
environmental factor might be afforded sign stimulus status by nothing more
than the selective sensitisation of the sensory organs. Thus a red-sensitive
retina might explain the red sensitive behaviour of the territorial stickleback,
and an elongated retina might explain an orientation
sensitivity. We therefore repeat our earlier quotation from Lorenz: "What
formerly was very simply conceived of as 'an instinct' is shown by the
advance of analysis to be a very complex mechanism of very distinct and very
different constituents, such as endogenous automatisms, releasing mechanisms,
taxes, kineses, and, maybe, quite a number of
further as yet unrecognised particulate functions" (Lorenz, 1950:261). This is why it is so important to follow Lloyd Morgan's canon, and always be on the look-out for the most parsimonious explanation of any single observation. Note also the curious phenomenon known as the supernormal stimulus. |
Social Order: Many organisms, including some of the very simplest, live in colonies. However this factor in isolation does not imply any internal structure to those colonies. A handful of maggots, for example, is an amorphous wriggling mass in which no one maggot is any more important than, or doing anything differently to, any other. The same number of ants or bees, however would have an intricate structure of different jobs to do, and the same number of mammals would have an intricate web of kin and dominance relationships. This is known as social order, and serves to maintain an optimum environment for courtship and reproduction. [For more on chimpanzee social order click here, and for an introduction to robot social order click here.] |
Supernormal Stimuli: Curiously enough, many sign stimulus investigations find that non-lifelike versions of a stimulus are actually more powerful releasers of the behaviour in question than are the corresponding naturally occurring stimuli. These are known as supernormal stimuli, and they provide yet another example of how instincts can be "fooled". In gulls, for example, egg-retrieval behaviour can easily be diverted from a real egg to a supernormal dummy; to a model which is literally "larger than life". |
Threat Gesture: A sign stimulus produced by one animal towards a conspecific in an attempt to claim dominance over it, and thus right by seniority to food or mating opportunity. May be followed either by a appeasement gesture, in which case that right is confirmed, or by ritualised combat, in which case it is fought over. [See more detailed discussion in Lesson 3.] |
Thwartings: A type of displacement activity [compare intrusions]. This is where the animal is physically prevented in engaging in a desired behaviour "A", but cannot prevent elements of behaviour "B" from occurring spontaneously. A thirsty dove, for example, will peck unnecessarily at the ground if it is prevented from getting to the water it is seeking. Similarly, Tinbergen (1952) describes how a male stickleback which has been courting an unreceptive female will suddenly break off, swim to the nesting site, and perform the characteristic egg-fanning movement of his fins which would otherwise not take place until well into the reproductive exchange. |
Vacuum Activity: The concept of vacuum activity (Leerlaufreaktion) was introduced by Lorenz (1939) to describe the emergence of behaviour in the total absence of any related stimulus. Birds, for example, when deprived of suitable nesting material, will nevertheless go to the nesting box and perform all the movements of carrying and placing material even though they have nothing in their beaks. Again, the suspicion is that the drive has been dammed up beyond some threshold level, after which parts of the behavioural sequence trigger themselves spontaneously and in a disorganised way. |
LESSON RATIONALE: And why does all this matter? Because humans are full of instincts (although they frequently do their best to hide them). Hence the next lesson .....
EXERCISES (AND STANDARD STUDY TIMES): Depending on how thoroughly you have been exploring the hyperlinks provided, it has probably taken you less than 30 minutes to read the foregoing text, and now you have to do some real work. Complete the following exercises, taking careful note of the expected study times: |
|
OC3.1.0 |
Browse the Internet, starting with the keywords <Lorenz Tinbergen ethology>, but probing ever further afield as other potential keywords take your eye. Look to build up a small e-folio of useful general commentary, recent research, and up-and-coming new theories (advertisements for forthcoming conferences are very useful in this respect). Study this information carefully, and you will suddenly find yourself as good at, if not better than, the experts in your organisation. Specialist reference archives are particularly valuable so note their location carefully. [No formal time limit.] |
Submitting Exercises for Assessment and Feedback (Fee-Paying Clients Only): Simply e-mail your answer(s) for full tutorial feedback. State each conclusion clearly, and briefly explain how you arrived at it. You may do this one exercise at a time, or all at once. Additional questions may then be asked, and additional tasks given as required. [Submit an Exercise] Please cooperate with this student-tutor exchange, because it will eventually form the basis of your individual student progress record. Do not proceed to Lesson OC3.2 until all the tutorial tasks are completed and signed off. |
Lesson OC3.2:
Human Ethology
Recommended
Reading: "The Naked Ape" Morris, D. (1994) To see an abstract, or to order this book, click here. |
|
So what about
humans? Is our behaviour, too, simply a function of hormonally induced drive
states and involuntary releasing ceremonies? Are we - despite all our lofty
pretensions - just rather large "animats",
or are we - and this is one of the most far-reaching questions facing modern
science - rational beings with minds of our own? We begin this line of
investigation by looking briefly at the "popular science" of body
language. This is an often over-marketed cousin of academic comparative
psychology, and it attempts to identify fragments of silent signalling -
or nonverbal communication - in our daily lives. Here are the main
points on the timeline:
·
Charles Darwin ("The Expression of the
Emotions in Man and Animals", 1872/1999). This was the first formal
attempt to trace humankind's biological inheritance, and listed a range of
emotional states - anxiety, joy, etc. - where both facial and postural behaviours
were "innate or inherited" (p348) and "the same throughout the
world" (p355).
·
Franz Boas (various, 1888-1944). Boas was the
anthropologist who popularised the use of photography as a research tool. He
began this work with still photography in 1894 and then moved on to movie film
in the 1930s, focusing on the use of dance and gesture as methods of
communication.
·
Margaret Mead ("Balinese Character: A
Photographic Analysis", 1942, as second author to Gregory Bateson;
"Coming of Age in Samoa", 1953). Mead was one of Boas's
students, and from 1925 conducted lengthy anthropological studies of Pacific
islanders. She concluded that in many respects Darwin had overstated the case
for emotional behaviours being the same throughout the world. There were
commonalities, true, but the final presentation of a given emotion was more the
result of cultural and environmental differences.
The 1972 ("Revised") Efron System of Gestural Classification
"Ideational"
Gestures: Gestures referring to the thought process itself
Speech Markers (also known as beats or baton signals:
gestures to emphasise stress, new item markers, chunkings
and groupings)
Ideographs (gestures sketching
in space a logical train of thought or paralleling abstract thinking; includes
metaphoric gestures, where the content consists of the story structure itself)
"Referential"
Gestures: Gestures referring to the external world
Depictive
Iconic (gestures of shape,
spatial relationship, or action)
Pantomimic (gestures miming
the role of the referent)
Evocative
Deictic (pointings)
Symbolic (also known as emblems:
gestures with a very precise but actually quite arbitrary meaning. Eg. the "Vee-sign" and similar insults)
From
Feldman and Rimé (1991:248; after Efron
(1941, 1972), but incorporating McNeill (1992:189).
The Intimate Distance: For
lovers and family, the personal space is about 6-18 inches.
The Personal Distance: For
close friends, the personal space is about 18-48 inches.
The Social Distance: For more
general relationships, the personal space is about 4-12 feet.
The Public Distance: For
public speaking, the personal space is 12 feet or more.
Whether
actively interacting, or as strangers momentarily sharing a location, people
position themselves at a psychologically comfortable distance from each other,
and it is acutely stressful if these rules of interaction are broken. Hall
(1959) named the science of personal space proxemics. Breaking the rules
of personal space is one of the three major symptoms of poor social skills (the
other two being poor conversational turn-taking and egocentrism). It is also
one of the commonest areas of misunderstanding in intercultural interaction
(see below). [For more specifically on proxemics, click here.]
·
Desmond Morris ("The Naked Ape", 1967;
"The Human Zoo", 1969). Following Darwin's lead, Morris's two
best-sellers looked for the biological roots of human behaviours in such areas
as social grouping, dominance and status, mate selection, sexual fetishisms,
and aggression. [To see Morris's website, click here.] Morris has also helped
popularise the analysis of coverbal gesture, although
as already noted the science is better credited formally to David Efron. Cassell (in press) reviews the recent academic
literature in this area.
·
Albert Mehrabian
("Tactics of Social Influence", 1970; "Silent Messages",
1971; "Nonverbal Communication", 1972). It was Mehrabian
who estimated (in Mehrabian, 1971) that visual
communication accounted for 55% of information transmission in a given
conversation, nonverbal auditory (eg. intonation) for
38%, and formal verbal content for a mere 7%, and who popularised (in Mehrabian, 1972) the term nonverbal communication (NVC).
·
Michael Watson ("Proxemic
Behaviour: A Cross-Cultural Study", 1970). Continuing the anthropological
tradition, Watson investigated cultural differences in proxemics, and found
that some cultures - eg. Arab, Latin American,
French, Italian, and Turkish - were "contact cultures" whereas others
- eg. Germany, England, Norway, Japan, Southeast
Asia, India, Pakistan, and the United States - were "noncontact
cultures". This showed in daily conversation, where a CC interaction would
be much more at ease with closer physical proximity, more body contact, and
longer periods of mutual gaze than an NN interaction, and where NC interactions
are typically uncomfortable and strained. Watson's work helped to usher in a
whole series of books about how people brought up in differing NVC environments
can communicate without constantly embarrassing themselves (and others) with faux
pas. Hall, Chia, and Wang (1996) have recently produced evidence that the
emotions of affection, anticipation, disgust, contempt, and acceptance are
among the harder ones to interpret cross-culturally. There are many good
sources of advice on this subject on the Internet, including Kevin Bucknall's notes on
Chinese business etiquette and Seligman (1999).
·
Ray Birdwhistell ("Kinesics
and context. Essays on body motion communication", 1970). Birdwhistell was another anthropologist, who in 1954 coined
the term kinesics to describe the moving as opposed to the static
aspects of NVC. After a long programme of cross-cultural observation, he
concluded that emotional expression was predominantly culturally transmitted.
[However, in the editorial to his 1999 edition of Darwin (1872/1999), Paul
Ekman describes in some detail why we should regard this conclusion as
academically unsafe.]
·
Julius Fast ("Body Language", 1971).
Fast was one of the first to exploit the commercial potential of the earlier
academic studies. "Body Language" was a pop-psychological best
seller, because it analysed the NVC of sexual attraction. Here we have the
issue of how men and women signal their interest in the opposite sex. Do we
follow simple courtship rules like sticklebacks, for example, or are our
choices more consciously determined? Many eye contact and postural signals have
been identified.
·
Displacement Activity: You can see suspicions of
displacement activity in such human activities as whistling, ear pulling, nose
scratching, lip licking, hand rubbing, cigarette lighting, key jangling, beard
stroking, eating and drinking, hair twiddling, and shrugging to relieve
tension. Such behaviours are particularly easy to observe in people dining
alone, for this is normally a group behaviour in
humans.
·
Sport: Desmond Morris ("The Soccer Tribe", 1981) has
speculated at length about a possible relationship between our history as
hunters and our liking for competitive sport. Why, for example, is it possible
so instantly and accurately to interpret the head-clasp of conceding a point
and the air-punch of scoring one? Similarly, Greer (1983) and Clayman (1993) have studied the effects of spectator booing
on team advantage.
·
Commercial Skills: Here we have the issue of whether
it is possible to increase the effectiveness of a commercial salesforce by
explicit training in NVC. Training regimes under this heading will usually be directed
at developing and then exploiting the psychological bond between the
salesperson and the potential customer. Other areas of commercial life where
NVC has a part to play are in the sciences of effective leadership, customer
relationship management, and corporate loyalty. These are so important that
entire lessons are devoted them later in this programme.
·
Career Development: A recent survey by the confidence
consultant Ros Taylor has highlighted the importance of
confidence in getting ahead. Being pushy, ambitious, arrogant, and - above all
- assertive, are far better predictors of success than ability to do the job (The
Daily Mail, 5th April 2001). [For further details, see Taylor (2000).]
·
Lie Detection (informally, "Pinocchio Science"): Here we have
the issue of how people can reveal dishonesty by involuntary facial and
postural signs (that is to say, by behavioural indicators rather than by
the physiological indicators utilised in the polygraph lie
detector).
·
Assault (Including Rape): There is a school of thought
which holds that at least some physical assaults, including rapes, result from
the victim somehow over-signalling submissiveness. Thus a
"mild-mannered" "nerd" might attract (or, more precisely,
not divert) more aggression than a "regular guy", and a behaviourally
naive female might look an easier target than a worldly wise one. Murzynski and Degelman (1996)
have shown, for example, that even such simply factors as fluency of movement
can affect the perception of vulnerability. [For a more detailed review, see Binder (1999).]
·
What Makes a Baby Attractive: Sternglantz, Gray, and Murakami (1977) showed undergraduates a variety
of baby-face line drawings in which chin size, eye width, eye height, and iris
size were varied systematically. They found that "cute" facial
features were reliably preferred, that there was little difference between male
and female preferences, and that what made for "cuteness" was a
balance between chin and forehead, and large eyes with medium-sized irises. The
authors note that one of the possible functions of cuteness is to defuse adult
male aggression, even to the extent that infants who are deficient in the
relevant dimensions might thereby be predisposed to battered child syndrome.
·
Autism and Eye Gaze: Both Hutt and Ounsted
(1966) and Tinbergen and Tinbergen (1972) have analysed the syndrome of early
infantile autism (Kanner's autism) in terms of
deficiencies in eye contact behaviour. Hutt and Ounsted
studied eight autistic children between three and six years old, and observed
that when wanting to be picked up by their carer they kept their faces averted.
Tinbergen and Tinbergen (1972) discussed the implications of Hutt and Ounsted's work, in the light of the child-stranger
interaction of normal children. Again, they looked especially at the phenomenon
of gaze aversion. In normal children two main response patterns were noted. One
was an immediately positive response of eye contact and smiling. The other was
a blank expression, aimed slightly past the adult's eyes. If pressed at this
juncture, the negatively responding child would then close its eyes for several
seconds, and, upon reopening them, either switch
immediately to a positive response or else intensify the negative one. Even in normals, therefore, a sufficiently stressful encounter can
provoke withdrawal behaviour: normal children are "very keen to establish
a social bond", but are prevented from doing so by "negative
tendencies" (p27). In autists, however, the negative tendencies somehow
get the upper hand.
·
Stuttering and Eye Gaze: Similarly, there have been
attempts to trace stuttering back to deficient eye contact behaviours. Lasalle and Conture (1991) found
that eye contact was significantly more frequent between young (mean 57.0
months) boy stutterers and their mothers during stuttering than for normals during fluency, and they suggested that this might
be due to two behaviours on the part of the mother. Firstly, a mother would
look to monitor her child's speech production, and secondly she would
look to inform him that he still had her attention. They warn, therefore,
that too much eye contact might be just as bad as too little.
·
Facial Imitation: Field, Woodson, Greenberg, and Cohen
(1982) studied the ability of 36-hour infants to imitate the facial expression
of their mothers, and found not only that adult facial expressions could be
differentiated (a presumption based upon the observation that they produced
different amounts of eye fixation), but that the equivalent infantile
imitations also followed more often than by chance. Thus the most common cause of an infantile widened lips was a happy adult face, while
the most common cause of a pout was a sad adult face, and the most common cause
of a wide open mouth was a surprised adult face. [For a detailed discussion of
facial imitation, click here.]
·
Smiling: Darwin himself (op. cit.) noted the ability of the human
infant's smile to elicit protective behaviour from adult carers. Freedman
(1964) reviewed work on the smiling responses of sighted, blind, deaf, and
blind-deaf human infants. In normals, a speaking
moving face is the most effective elicitor of smiling, and the peak response
period is between three and six months. In blind infants, there are differences
but these only tend to occur after several years, and can be attributed to the
lack of opportunity to imitate visually. The main early difference was that the
smiles of blind infants are fleeting and "reflex-like". All impaired
infants start to smile to social stimulation at about the same age as normals, "which indicates that no single sensory
channel is the exclusive releaser of smiling" (op. cit., p182).
·
Facial Emotion: Ekman, Friesen, and Ellsworth (1972)
review the evidence for cross-cultural universals in the ability to recognise
facial emotion. They studied the emotional judgements of observers from 14
widely different cultures, and found "conclusive" evidence for at
least some universals. Happiness, fear, surprise, anger, disgust, and sadness
are particularly highly correlated. They stress, however, that this is not
final proof of innate neural "programs", because common learning
experience alone would be a sufficient explanation. Rinn
(1991) is an excellent source on the neuropsychology of facial expressions, if interested, including those activated by fixed actions
patterns. [To experiment with some build-it-yourself cartoon faces click here.]
·
Laughter: Jason Rutter, of the University of
Salford, has studied the interaction between performers and their audiences
during the delivery of humour. He sees laughter as being invited by the speaker
from his audience in much the same way that pauses signal turn-taking in
conventional conversation. [For some positively fascinating examples, click here.]
·
The Eyes: Guthrie (1970) points out that only
in humans are the whites of the eyes (the sclera) visible. In all other
mammals only the pupil and iris can readily be seen. This is because the sclera play an important role in communication. "We
have an uncanny ability," he says, "to determine the exact position
of an individual's stare even though he is on the other side of the room -
merely from judging the symmetrical alignment of a round pattern (iris) on a
spherical one (eyeball). Exposure of the white sclera aids considerably in that
ability. [The sclera] allow the transmission of fairly precise signals from the
eyes." (pp268-269.) [For more recent reviews of
this area, click here.]
·
Drama, Public Speaking, and Political Oratory: There are a
number of quite diverse aspects to this area of study. Bougehold
(1999), for example, has studied the use of postural and gestural coverbals in Greek literature, and concludes that many
rather obscure texts were written primarily for public performance and make
little sense without knowledge of the body language used during that
performance. Canada's Opera Atelier website takes this further, giving explicit stage directions to performers
wishing to convey particular emotions to their audiences without using words
[for examples, click here]. Similarly, Peter Bull of
the University of York has shown that politicians rely heavily on
gesturally-assisted rhetorical devices. In one paper (Bull, 1990), he studied
videoed party political speeches and found that most gestures either coincided
exactly with a vocal stress point or emphasised it in some direct way.
All in all, the
formal science of human ethology and the popular science of body language have
uncovered many fascinating issues, even if the academic jury is still awaiting
conclusive evidence on many of them. But the award for the most sustained
research programme of all goes without doubt to the work of Irenäus
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, Honorary Director of the Ludwig-Boltzmann Institute for Urban
Ethology in Vienna. As a young man, Professor Eibl-Eibesfeldt studied under
Konrad Lorenz himself, and began his academic career researching animal
behaviour [to see the polecat story again, click here]. He then used the
skills he had acquired to make a hard science out of human ethology. His
achievements are summarised in a private subscription library of video and
audio cameos sponsored by the Max Planck Society and the University of Munich.
[To see Eibl-Eibesfeldt's homepage click here,
and for more on the study resource proper click here.]
LESSON RATIONALE: And why does all this matter? Because there is a growing recognition that both personal and commercial success (not to mention the future of the entire human race) depends upon our being in touch with, and therefore being able to manage, our animal inheritance. Hence the next lesson .....
EXERCISES (AND STANDARD STUDY TIMES): Depending on how thoroughly you have been exploring the hyperlinks provided, it has probably taken you less than 30 minutes to read the foregoing text, and now you have to do some real work. Complete the following exercises, taking careful note of the expected study times: |
|
OC3.2.0 |
Browse the Internet, starting with the keywords <gesture language> and <social skill>, but probing ever further afield as other potential keywords take your eye. Look to build up a small e-folio of useful general commentary, recent research, and up-and-coming new theories (advertisements for forthcoming conferences are very useful in this respect). Study this information carefully, and you will suddenly find yourself as good at, if not better than, the experts in your organisation. Specialist reference archives are particularly valuable so note their location carefully. [No formal time limit.] |
OC3.2.1 |
Access the facial expression site at [click here], and set the parameters to print off the expression of your boss (a) when angry, and (b) when pleased. [1 hour.] |
OC3.2.2 |
Discretely observe the behaviour at a busy commercial service point (eg. cash machine or check-out). What displacement behaviours can be identified when someone is beaten to the queue, and do these behaviours differ with age, ethnicity, or gender? [2 hours.] |
OC3.2.3 |
Discretely observe the behaviour at a restaurant table or in a cafeteria queue. What vacuum activities or intention movements can be identified when someone is getting hungry but is not yet able to start eating, and do these behaviours differ with age, ethnicity or gender? [2 hours.] |
OC3.2.4 |
In small groups, take it in turns to playact the behaviour by which you would signal silent apologies to a senior male speaker upon being late to enter his presentation. Repeat for a junior male speaker, and both senior and junior female speakers. [1 hour.] |
OC3.2.5 |
In small groups, take it in turns to playact the behaviour by which you would signal that you need to leave an important meeting to answer a call of nature. Would it make a difference if opposite sex colleagues were present? [1 hour.] |
OC3.2.6 |
In small groups, take it in turns to playact the behaviour by which you would signal that you are in a hurry and would like to bring a conversation with a senior male to a close. Repeat for a junior male, and both senior and junior females. [1 hour.] |
OC3.2.7 |
[If circumstances permit] Discretely observe a new arrival in an organisation for evidence of imprinting. [2 hours.] |
OC3.2.8 |
[If circumstances permit] While a same-sex colleague is away from his/her desk, start working at it yourself. Carefully note your colleague's reactions when s/he returns. Repeat for an opposite-sex colleague, noting any gender differences in response. [1 hours.] |
OC3.2.9 |
[COMMERCIALLY IMPORTANT] Discretely observe your organisation's customer facing operations (eg. lobby, helpdesk, or point of sale). What fears and uncertainties can you detect in a person approaching for the first time? [2 hours.] CONTINUED ........... |
OC3.2.10 |
[COMMERCIALLY IMPORTANT] ..... How are those potential new customers then made welcome? (That is to say, how are their fears and uncertainties dispelled?) Or are they ignored, and a potentially major sales opportunity lost? [2 hours.] |
Submitting Exercises for Assessment and Feedback (Fee-Paying Clients Only): Simply e-mail your answer(s) for full tutorial feedback. State each conclusion clearly, and briefly explain how you arrived at it. You may do this one exercise at a time, or all at once. Additional questions may then be asked, and additional tasks given as required. [Submit an Exercise] Please cooperate with this student-tutor exchange, because it will eventually form the basis of your individual student progress record. Do not proceed to Lesson OC3.3 until all the tutorial tasks are completed and signed off. |
Lesson
OC3.3: Social Order and Aggression
Recommended Reading: "Grooming, Gossip, and the
Evolution of Language" Dunbar, R. (1997) To see an abstract, or to order this book, click here |
|
In Lesson 2, we
saw that many ethological concepts and terms can profitably be applied to human
behaviour as well as to animal behaviour. However, we deliberately deferred our
main discussion of social order and aggression until now, because the linked
topics of dominance, threat, and submission are important enough to warrant a
lesson on their own.
The entry level
concept here is that of the pecking order, an idea which derives from an
analysis of the social behaviour of chickens by the Norwegian psychologist Schjelderup-Ebbe (1922). He noted that one hen literally
ruled the roost, being at the top of a complex hierarchy (in German, Hackordnung) of who was "allowed" to peck
whom. This sets up the sort of dominance hierarchy described in Lesson
1. Similar pecking orders exist (mutatis mutandis) in many mammal
species, and are common, in particular, in carnivores and primates.
The problem with
pecking orders, however, is that pecking, biting, and scratching can hurt. The
beak, teeth, and claws are weapons, and if a species is to thrive it cannot
allow too many potential reproducers to be killed unnecessarily. The pecking
order therefore needs to be very closely policed to ensure that actual injury
is rare, and the upshot is that animals are frequently "all mouth and no
trousers": they shout (or charge, or butt, or lock antlers, or snarl,
etc.), but are ready to back off if (a) appeased (whereupon they win), or (b)
the rival shouts back louder (whereupon they lose).
Here is a brief
review of the mechanisms by which the dominance hierarchy is maintained in
humans:
·
Physical Challenge: This is usually consists of a rapid
forward lunge, or an attempt, or feigned attempt, at striking or kicking.
·
Vocalisation: Dominance can also be asserted by
screeching, screaming, and shouting. If delivered from a distance, this will
often be accompanied by movements towards the object of attention, and if
delivered face-to-face will be accompanied by a forward jaw thrust so as to
deliberately violate the other person's personal space.
·
Eyes and Eyebrows: The eyes and eyebrows are extremely
versatile organs of nonverbal communication, being able to deliver an intimidatory stare as well as signalling submission.
Indeed, movements of the brow play a part in communicating many emotional
messages. By and large a thick brow serves to give the underlying structure a
more massive appearance. Guthrie (1970) also notes that human females 'make up'
artificial eyebrows at a raised position of appeasement and attentiveness.
·
Lips and Teeth: These are used in two ways. In the
angry face, the upper lip is drawn very tight to relatively low corners, and
the jaw is slightly open and stiffened to display our rather puny canine teeth.
In the fear face, on the other hand, the jaw is tightly clenched, the upper lip
is a lot slacker, and the corners of the mouth are drawn up by tension in the
cheek muscles. This serves to show more teeth. The fear face is believed by
many to be the evolutionary forerunner of the smile.
·
Looming and Slumping: Many long animals threaten by
turning broadside on (a behaviour often used to deter would-be predators as
well), and emphasising one's height is regularly part of the threat display
repertoire in ground-dwelling species. Riskind (1984)
points to one interesting residue of such behaviours in humans. He asks (p479):
"Why do many people seem to slump or shrink, almost as if they are hiding,
when they face personal failures? Why do many seem to rise up and physically
expand in height and size when they face personal gains or triumphs?" He
found that slumping not only follows some sort of perceived setback, but that
if it is artificially induced in a success situation it seriously erodes the
person's perception of that success. Indeed, there are several height-lowering
submission behaviours, such as bowing and cringing. Guthrie (op. cit.) even
points to the way such height-related concepts can enter spoken language in
such phrases as "your highness", "stuck-up", "feeling
low", etc. Sheer physical size is also important in deciding whether to
challenge a threat gesture or to submit to it. However, this is not true of the
territorial defence challenges of sticklebacks (see Lesson 1). If a big
stickleback strays into the territory of a small one, the small one will not
hesitate in attempting to drive it off, and the big one will accept the signal
and back away. The aggression in this case is more the assertion of "first
come, first served" than of dominance, and there must therefore be quite
significant genetic programming differences between stickleback threat displays
and, say, wolf or baboon threat displays.
·
Scalp Hair (And Lack Thereof): Scalp hair
can be erected into a threat crest in many species. This can be done
bilaterally or medially. Grey and receding hair, on the other hand, is not a
totally negative sign, in that it commands some authority by virtue of the
maturity and wisdom which go with it.
·
The Beard and Chin: Prominent beards "arose as
structures of aggression used by mature males to intimidate other mature
males" (Guthrie, 1970:262). In many primates and a number of non-primates,
"the chin and cheek whiskers increase the apparent mass of the lower face
- the area important to animals that fight and communicates with their
mouths" (ibid.). As a result, it is aggressive to jut out the chin, and
submissive to drop it down towards the chest.
·
Social Grooming Theory: Inferiors in a social
hierarchy often ingratiate themselves with their superiors by grooming them.
They remain inferior, but they become preferred inferiors; seconds and thirds
in command rather than out-and-out leaders. Robin Dunbar of Liverpool
University has developed an interesting theory of how and why this behaviour
has shaped not just modern human society, but the modern human brain as well
(Dunbar, 1993). Noting that grooming behaviour went on far longer than was
strictly necessary for the removal of ticks and burrs, he hypothesised that it
must be serving some greater purpose than mere mutual cleansing, specifically
maintaining group coherence. He called this phenomenon social grooming.
He then saw the entire process of human evolution as relying on three
interacting and co-evolving factors, namely brain size, group size, and language.
Language, he argued, was simply a more efficient form of social grooming than
fur picking: true it required a bigger brain, but it enabled a larger, and
therefore more globally competent, and therefore more successful, social group.
So
much for vertical social order - the dominance hierarchy - but what about
horizontal social order? What
about family versus family, or tribe versus tribe, or nation versus nation, or
race versus race? How is it - and this is perhaps the
most urgent of all humanity's burning issues - if overt intraspecific
aggression is so rare in nature, that humans keep killing each other by the
million? No other group of animals is more ruthless in its aggression than
adult humans, with Richardson (1960) estimating that in the period 1820-1945
there were at least 59 million war-related human deaths. As to why this should
be, one common explanation is that humans have learned to kill at a distance,
so that their aggression can no longer be moderated by any submission ritual.
Human wars are rarely face-to-face affairs. Instead, they are
concept-to-concept or emotion-to-emotion in minds often many thousands of miles
apart. This is why a soldier can offer a cigarette to a prisoner whom he had
been trying to kill only a few minutes previously. It is also why weapons of
mass destruction delivered by pushing an anonymous "missile away"
button have always posed such a threat.
This is a
promising and recurring theme. In small tribal groups [to see how Australian
aborigines ritualise their disagreements, click
here], man's aggression can be
appropriately controlled, but the change away from that way of life has happened
too quickly for the biological protections to co-evolve. In his 1969 book The
Human Zoo, Morris gives us the concept of the "supertribe",
that is to say, a social unit bigger than the stone age
tribal unit, and sees the problem as going back into the neolithic
period of civilisation, thus:
"By
our standards the Sumerian cities were small, with populations ranging from
7000 to no more than 20000. Nevertheless, our simple tribesman had already come
a long way. He had become a citizen, a super-tribesman, and the key difference
was that in a super-tribe he no longer knew personally every member of his
community. It was this change, the shift from the personal to the
impersonal society, that was going to cause the human
animal its greatest agonies in the millennia ahead. As a species we were not
biologically equipped to cope with a mass of strangers masquerading as members
of our tribe." (Morris, 1969:20; emphasis original.)
Dunbar's social
grooming theory (see above) also has a lot to say on this. Dunbar plots group
size against the ability to "service" the increasing number of
relationships involved. Other things being equal, he calculated that a
200-person human group would have to devote 56.6% of its day to manual social
grooming, just to maintain that workable group, and this would gravely curtail
its ability to do all the other things - hunting, building, cooking, etc. -
needing to be done. With language, however, the social grooming is done in
seconds rather than minutes, with brief social greetings (eg.
"hello-how-are-you") delivered more or less in passing.
So the problems
only arise when civilisation moved man into groupings larger than he was used
to, and more complex perhaps than he was (or still is) psychologically and
behaviourally ready for. With civilisation comes the problem that you no longer
know all your neighbours. Moreover, you get new rivalries between separate
civilisations themselves. As a result, if you are "in-group" you are
OK, but if you are "out-group" then you had better beware. In this
way, all modern conflict can be reduced to this insufficiency of appeasement
behaviour in the face of a very dangerous propensity for developing different
belief systems. Zegans (1971) puts it this way:
"The human thus appears unique among primates in that man will die for
symbols and slaughter for abstractions while often ignoring the biological
survival needs of his own people" (p359). Even the clothes on our backs
have been held to be means of aggression and social order. Thus:
"All
male costume tends to become a uniform, by which is meant not something which
is worn by everyone, but something that can be worn only by certain people.
Once any kind of civilisation has been established we find a whole system of
uniforms. The King has a special dress, so has the Priest. In this sense, the
dress of all men of a certain social rank is a uniform." (Laver, 1964:102.)
".....in
wars or clashes between clans man often uses artificial supersymbols
of threat, almost always modeled after the ancestral
devices: the bearskin hat, warpaint, the war bonnet,
plumes, brilliantly coloured attire, and very loud sounds." (Guthrie, 1970:297).
"The
uniform [worn by offenders in a correctional institution] can reinforce group
cohesion and provide a common identity and role perspective among those in
uniform. As a potential consequence, however, it can also alienate
non-uniformed staff ....." (Correctional Service of
Canada, Report B-02, February 1989).
And
Eibl-Eibesfeldt (1974) explains how teasing and mocking behaviour also has a
distinct social purpose. "By teasing and mocking," he
writes, "group homogeneity is enforced and an outlet for aggression
provided. [] Mocking is done by imitating the patterns which provoked the
hostility of the group and thus ridiculing it" (Eibl-Eibesfeldt,
1974:452). On a smaller scale, Charlie Chaplin, it seems, would mercilessly
pantomime the idiosyncrasies of any ingénue who irritated him.
The whole of Study
Unit OC4 [NOT YET AVAILABLE] will be devoted to the depths to which humans can
descend in times of intergroup rivalry, out-and-out war, or even in the privacy
of their own front room, that is to say, to the "darker side" of
humanity; the time when things go wrong with the delicately balanced
relationship between our minds and our animal selves.
LESSON RATIONALE: And why does all this matter? Because organisations are microcosms - societies in miniature. They have similar pecking orders, and similar affiliation and conflict groupings, and only if these natural tendencies are properly understood and allowed for will the overall organisation be healthy. We return to this issue in Unit OC4.
EXERCISES (AND STANDARD STUDY TIMES): Depending on how thoroughly you have been exploring the hyperlinks provided, it has probably taken you less than 30 minutes to read the foregoing text, and now you have to do some real work. Complete the following exercises, taking careful note of the expected study times: |
|
OC3.3.0 |
Browse the Internet, starting with the keywords <social order dominance>, but probing ever further afield as other potential keywords take your eye. Look to build up a small e-folio of useful general commentary, recent research, and up-and-coming new theories (advertisements for forthcoming conferences are very useful in this respect). Study this information carefully, and you will suddenly find yourself as good at, if not better than, the experts in your organisation. Specialist reference archives are particularly valuable so note their location carefully. [No formal time limit.] |
OC3.3.1 |
List as many characteristics of a dominant person as you can. Study the behaviour of a WWF (World Wrestling Federation) wrestler during the ritual pre-bout challenge. How many biological dominance signals can you identify, and how well are they being faked. [1 hour.] |
OC3.3.2 |
Ask (a) a male friend, and (b) a female friend, to interact with you over a period of time wearing mirrored sunglasses. Record any experiences of discomfort. Ask (a) a male friend, and (b) a female friend, to stand proudly. Describe all postural and gestural displays, and record any sex differences which emerge. [2 hours.] |
OC3.3.3 |
Estimate how many people you know (a) by sight, (b) by nodding acquaintance, (c) by name, (d) to talk to, but not as family or friends, and (e) family and friends. Dunbar says that (d) and (e) added together should be about 200. Is this right? [1 hour.] |
OC3.3.4 |
Estimate how many people you would happily lend £100 to. Then identify five you would not feel safe lending money to, and state why not. [1 hour. Keep the identities of the distrusted five to yourself for legal reasons.] |
OC3.3.5 |
[COMMERCIALLY IMPORTANT] Study the academic literature for introductory material on authoritarianism, paternalism, and dominance and deference in professional interaction. [2 hours.] What issues does this research raise, and what implications do these issues have for a healthy organisational communication? [5 hours. You may submit a short essay under the title "Does authoritarian management work?" if you prefer.] |
OC3.3.6 |
[COMMERCIALLY IMPORTANT] Despite all the problems, pecking orders actually work (and have done for several hundred million years). Suggest how this deeply ingrained preference for clear hierarchical structures might legislate against the modern trend towards "leaner flatter" organisations and self-managing teams. [5 hours. You may submit a short essay under the title "Do employees need hierarchy?" if you prefer.] |
Submitting Exercises for Assessment and Feedback (Fee-Paying Clients Only): Simply e-mail your answer(s) for full tutorial feedback. State each conclusion clearly, and briefly explain how you arrived at it. You may do this one exercise at a time, or all at once. Additional questions may then be asked, and additional tasks given as required. [Submit an Exercise] Please cooperate with this student-tutor exchange, because it will eventually form the basis of your individual student progress record. Do not proceed until all the tutorial tasks are completed and signed off. |
So
Where To Next?
If you got to this point by mistake, click to return
as appropriate:
Otherwise,
congratulations!! You have reached the end of Unit OC3 of the ORGANISATIONAL
COMMUNICATION programme.
Beaty, D.
(1969). The Human Factor in Aircraft
Accidents. London: Secker and Warburg.
Birdwhistell, R. (1970). Kinesics
and Context: Essays on Body Motion Communication. Philadephia,
PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.
Bougehold, A.L. (1999). When
a Gesture was Expected: A Selection of Examples from
Archaic and Classical Greek Literature. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University
Press.
Cannon, W.B. (1927). The thalamic
theory of emotion. American Journal of Psychology, 39:115-124.
Carthy,
J.D. and Ebling, F.J. (Eds). (1964). The Natural History of Aggression. London:
Academic Press.
Clayman, S.E. (1993). Booing: The anatomy of a disaffiliative
response. American Sociological Review, 58:110-130.
Darwin, C. (1872). The Expression
of the Emotions in Man and Animals. London: Murray. [Page numbering
from the readily available 1999 reprint
(incorporating editorial commentary by Paul Ekman), London: Harper
Collins.]
Dunbar, R.I.M.
(1993). Coevolution of neocortical size, group size, and
language in humans. Behavioural and Brain Sciences,
16(4):681-726.
Efron, D. (1941). Gesture, Race, and
Culture. New York: King's Crown.
Efron, D. (1972). Gesture and
Environments. The Hague / Paris: Mouton.
Eibl-Eibesfeldt,
I. (1961). The interactions of unlearned behaviour patterns
and learning in mammals. In Delafresnaye,
J.F. (Ed). Brain Mechanisms and Learning.
Oxford: Blackwell.
Eibl-Eibesfeldt,
I. (1974) The myth of the aggression-free hunter and
gatherer society. In Holloway, R.L. (Ed). Primate Aggression, Territoriality, and Xenophobia.
New York: Academic Press.
Emery,
N.J. (2000). The eyes have it: The
neuroethology, function, and evolution of social gaze. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews, 24:581-604.
Freeman, D.
(1964). Human aggression in anthropological perspective.
In The Natural History of Aggression. Carthy, J.D. and
Ebling, F.J. (Eds).
London: Academic Press.
Goodall, J.
(1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Greer, D. (1983). Spectator booing and the home advantage. Social
Psychology Quarterly, 46:252-261.
Guhl, A.M. (1956). The social order of
chickens. Scientific American, February 1956.
Guthrie, R.D.
(1970). Evolution of human threat display organs. Evolutionary Biology,
4:257-302.
Hall,
C.W., Chia, R., and Wang, D.F. (1996). Nonverbal communication among American and Chinese
students. Psychological Reports, 79:419-428.
Hall, E.T. (1959).
The Silent Language. New York: Doubleday.
Hall, E.T. (1966).
The Hidden Dimension. New York: Doubleday.
Harrison-Matthews,
L. (1964). Overt fighting in mammals. In The Natural History of Aggression. Carthy, J.D. and
Ebling, F.J. (Eds).
London: Academic Press.
Jones, I.H. (1971).
Stereotyped aggression in a group of Australian Western
Desert aborigines. British Journal of Medical Psychology,
44:259-265.
Kling,
A. and Mass, R. (1974) Alterations of social behaviour with neural lesions in
nonhuman primates. In Holloway, R.L. (Ed). Primate
Aggression, Territoriality, and Xenophobia. New York: Academic
Press.
Klopper, A.
(1964). Physiological background to aggression. In The Natural History of Aggression. Carthy, J.D. and
Ebling, F.J. (Eds).
London: Academic Press.
Laver,
J. (1964). Costume
as a means of social aggression. In The Natural
History of Aggression. Carthy, J.D. and Ebling, F.J. (Eds). London: Academic Press.
Lorenz, K.Z.
(1937). Über die Bildung
des Instinktbegriffs. Naturwissenschaften,
25:289-331.
Lorenz, K.Z.
(1939). Vergleichende Verhaltensforschung.
Zoologische Anzeiger.
Supplement (Verhandlungen, Deutsche Zoologische Gesellschaft,
41), 12:69-102.
Lorenz, K.Z.
(1943). Die angeborenen Formen
möglicher Ehrfahrung. Zeitschrift fürTierpsychologie,
5:235-409.
Lorenz, K.Z. (1950).
The comparative method in studying innate behaviour patterns.
In Physiological Mechanisms in Animal Behaviour.
Danielli, J.F. and Brown R. (Eds).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Lorenz, K.Z.
(1952). King Solomon's Ring. London: Methuen. [Page
numbers from the 1953 version published by the Reprint Society.]
Lorenz, K.Z.
(1958). The evolution of behaviour. Scientific
American, December 1958.
Lorenz, K.Z.
(1964). Ritualised fighting. In The
Natural History of Aggression. Carthy, J.D. and Ebling, F.J. (Eds). London: Academic Press.
Mehrabian, A. (1970). Tactics of Social
Influence. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Mehrabian, A. (1971). Silent Messages.
Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
Mehrabian, A. (1972). Nonverbal
Communication. Chicago, IL: Aldine-Atherton.
Melchers,
M. (1963). Zur Biologie
und zum Verhalten von Cupiennius salei.
Zoologische Jahrbucher,
91:1-90.
Mikanovicz, C.K. and Shank, S.L. (2000). [Electronic Document] Strategies
for developing communication between nurses and physicians. http://www.nursece.com/Communication.htm
[accessed 8th December 2000]
Morris, D. (1969).
The Human Zoo. London: Cape.
Murzynski, J. and Degelman, D.
(1996). Body
language of women and judgements of vulnerability to sexual assault. Journal
of Applied Social Psychology, 26:1617-1626.
Quarantelli, E.L. (1954). The nature and
conditions of panic. American Journal of Sociology, 60:267-275.
Rinn, W.E. (1991). Neuropsychology of
facial expression. In Fundamentals of nonverbal
behaviour. Feldman, R.S. and Rimé,
B. (Eds.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. (jointly
with Editions de la Maison des Sciences de l'Homme).
Riskind, J.H. (1984). They stoop to conquer: Guiding and
self-regulatory functions of physical posture after success and failure. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 47:479-493.
Scott,
J.P. (1974) Agonistic behaviour of primates. In Holloway, R.L. (Ed). Primate Aggression, Territoriality, and Xenophobia.
New York: Academic Press.
Spalding, D.
(1873) Instinct. Macmillan's Magazine, 27:282-293. (Reprinted
in British Journal of Animal Behaviour, 1954, 2:1-11.)
Sternglantz, S.H., Gray, J.L., and
Murakami, M. (1977). Adult
preferences for infantile facial features: an ethological approach. Animal
Behaviour, 25:108-115.
Sutton,
D., Larson, C., and Lindeman, R.C. (1974). Neocortical and limbic lesion effects on primate
phonation. Brain Research, 71:61-75.
Zegans, L.S. (1971). Towards a unified
theory of human aggression. British Journal of Medical Psychology,
44:355-365.